A slightly different version of this review was published yesterday at Substance News.
Paul Tough, KIPP, and the Character Con: A Review of How Children Succeed: Grit, Curiosity, and the Hidden Power of Character
Jim Horn
Tough’s liberal framing of
Paul Ryan’s goal to teach America’s urban poor good discipline and character is
imbued with a thinly-veiled social engineering urge that is growing stronger by
the day. It is based on a messy
collection of far-fetched extrapolations from a science-seeming psychological
intervention that is now being employed in the education reform schools by non-professionals
who haven’t the foggiest notion of what kind of damage they are doing to the
children of the poor who are, otherwise, being “cured” of the ill effects of
poverty. Tough is a good writer, too good to have written such an implausible
promo for the kinds of neo-eugenic ministrations that he supports in this book
as a solution to the “character problem” and the “executive functioning”
problem of those among us who are now viewed by elites in much the same way
that the grandfathers of Tough’s generation might have viewed the “culturally
deprived” version of the “white man’s burden” during the 1950s and 1960s.
There are significant
differences between then and now, however, and the most obvious one has to do
with money. In the 1960s, the War on
Poverty poured billions into job training programs and educational
interventions like Title I, all aimed to ameliorate some of the grosser
educational inequities with programs that left in place structural inequalities
like segregated housing that were viewed by liberals as too entrenched and
controversial to garner support for changing. Today poverty rates are higher
than when President Johnson declared the War on Poverty in 1964, but the
preferred solution over economic intervention is a thoroughly unproven and
un-researched form of psychological programming for children that hopes to
excavate new roadways in the cognitive maps of young brains in order to
inoculate them from the effects of poverty, which remains a problem too
expensive to fix for the elites who advocate this new form of eugenics. Today’s concentration of social efficiency
goals would make the advocates of scientific management a hundred years ago
blush, with even the charity by the rich, now called venture philanthropy, turned
into tax-sheltered investments with immediate returns as well as a big future
payoff. Cashing in down the road will come
as a result of the systematic indoctrination of a whole generation of urban
poor, to be accomplished through corporate reform schools that are given free
rein to run their psychological experiments on America’s most vulnerable
children.
The old eugenics of our
great-grandfathers advanced the belief that those deemed a threat to the health
of society, by way of inheritable behaviors, mental disorders, character
malfunctions, and physical disabilities, had defective “germ plasm” that could
not be improved but, rather, needed curtailing from further reproduction in
offspring. That “crisis” led to 30-plus states passing mandatory sterilization
laws that produced over 60,000 individual sterilizations. The unfinished legacy of that dark episode
lives on, with victims even today hoping to be somehow compensated for their
sexual maiming during another “progressive” era that eagerly embraced social steering
based on a crackpot pseudo-science over the rights of its most vulnerable
citizens.
The new eugenics is not
nearly so pessimistic about changing the defective character of the poor. In fact, today’s equally arrogant and
misguided scheme to save poor children from their defective character is full of
positivity, you might say, from the guru of positive psychology, himself—Dr.
Martin Seligman. Seligman’s pioneering
work on learned helplessness and learned optimism are central, in fact, to the
carving of new cognitive tracks in the malleable brains of elementary and
middle school children, whose structural plasticity lends itself well to
alternating jolts of learned helplessness and learned optimism—the mainstays of
the new and improved post-New Age version of breaking ‘em down to build ‘em up.
And what kind of character
flaws is Tough trying to find scientific justification for altering? Well, it’s not children’s moral character
that the white missionary girls from TFA are trying their darnedest to improve
in these charter reform schools like KIPP; it is, rather, something Tough calls
“performance character,” which is a distillation of the more expansive list of
traits developed by Seligman and Peterson that, if developed fully, will create
an unending psychological drip of happiness memes. Think of it as if the power of an invisible hand
had writ these qualities on every poor child’s digital tablet to carry around
with them and apply as each situation demands:
grit, self-control, zest, social intelligence, gratitude, optimism,
curiosity (p. 76). Gone are those
old-school character traits such as honesty, integrity, loving thy cubicle
companion, etc. Tough, in fact, attempts
to make the case that moral character is based on moral law that, by necessity,
is imposed by some higher authority. In
following Seligman and Peterson, Tough claims “moral laws were limiting when it
came to character because they reduced virtuous conduct to a simple matter of
obedience to a higher authority” (p. 59).
Moral character is traded
in, then, for “performance character.” The
new “performance character” traits that KIPP co-founder, David Levin, has boiled
down for child consumption focuses first and foremost on “grit,” which is to
say the individual possession of a kind of crusty abrasiveness or personality
pumice that may turn any barrier into “zest,” we might suppose. According to Tough, Levin believes his
approach stands far above any charge of cultural colonialism because “the
character-strength approach is…fundamentally devoid of value judgment” (p.
60). Tough doesn’t bother to explain how
Levin’s derived values of grit, self-control, and gratitude are any less of an
imposition than, let’s say, wisdom, justice, and temperance. We may only surmise that Levin’s blindness to
his own imposition of corporate ethics is intended to cloak any sign of force
feeding for the children who are imposed upon daily to view their own
mistreatment as an indicator that they, themselves, are not working hard or
being good enough to be treated with dignity.
The drawing below is a copy of a worksheet that Seligman disciple,
Angela Duckworth, has used in developing the performance character curriculum
for poor kids in Philadelphia schools.
Note that children are taught to swallow and digest the abuse handed
down by authority figures whose verbal assaults are to be viewed as sure signs of
their care for children who must do better in order to avoid what children believe is deserved denigration (click image to enlarge).
Tough’s book is out to promote an abusively-deployed variety of moral colonialism disguised as character building, even if he takes the most roundabout way to reach the conclusion from which he started, which may be stated thusly for those disinclined to read the book: KIPP and the education reform schools like it are doing the necessary work to save non-privileged, defective cultures from their own defective character, by making their children salable commodities in the future job market through a psychological regimen we would call brainwashing if it were used by a drug-crazed preacher in a faraway jungle like Guyana. Tough’s defense of the new eugenics is built upon a shaky collection of scientific tidbits and inappropriate analogies to other more humane interventions that Tough apparently believes are not unalike the pedagogical brutality and mind alteration practices that occur in the corporate reform schools such as KIPP. For instance, Tough analogizes from his experiences reporting on New York City’s chess champion school, IS 318, and through a number of examples of single-minded obsession paying off in the world of chess, suggests there is some lesson that may be transferred to understand KIPP’s 10,000 hours of total compliance test preparation and psychological throttling. As in other examples that Tough employs in his book, there are more differences between KIPP’s academic rigor mortis and becoming a chess champ than there are similarities.
The first and most glaring
difference has to do with a significant word that Tough advocates for and uses
a good deal in the book—volition. Now
while the great chess champions do, indeed, choose at some point in their lives
to be champions, as Scottish grandmaster, Jonathan Rowson, exemplifies in
Tough’s retelling, the children of KIPP do not choose an education based on an
unyielding behavioral catechism enforced by non-psychologists imposing
behavioral-cognitive treatments to produce total compliance and complicity by
children in their own subjugation. Secondly,
if the single-minded pursuit of becoming a grand master allows one to conclude
that “chess is a creative and beautiful pursuit” and a “celebration of
existential freedom,” what does the manipulative pursuit of scholar dollars
teach, or years of enforced silence, or the thousands of hours of lockdown test
prep accompanied by endless reams of mindless worksheets? Is there some celebration of existential
freedom in this “productive” performance of KIPPsterism?
Thirdly, if 10,000 hours of
chess practice makes one a master, what does 10,000 hours of KIPP provide? A slave who has learned how to accept her
bondage, along with a mediocre score that earns a seat in a third-tier
university where the odds are 5:1 of washing out? This college outcome remains a stubborn fact,
even with KIPP’s river of tax-sheltered cash to provide counselors, college
academic coaches, and other interventions to raise the KIPP college graduation
rate. Fourthly, the “joyousness” that results from chess is produced by the analysis
of a problem and application of a solution from a repertoire of proven
solutions or novel ones. How is this at
all analogous to the entirely rule-bound and low-level imitative and rote anesthesia
of KIPP learning? Tough offers no clues but
seems to believe that sitting these two very different phenomena side by side will
somehow lead readers to assume they are similar.
Tough does something similar
at other places in the book. For
instance, the subject of stress reduction among children of impoverished
families occupies a significant section in the first half of the book. Stress levels are closely tied to “executive
functioning” among children, and stress overloads are predictive of low
executive functioning. Since the improvement
of “executive functioning,” as defined by increased memory skills, is the Holy
Grail of the education reform industry, the industry has to pay attention to
stress. According to Tough’s
interpretation of the research literature, however, it is not really stress
that is the problem but, rather, the body’s response to stress. Just as poverty is not the problem, but the
stressed reactions to poverty: “It
wasn’t poverty itself that was compromising the executive function abilities of
the poor kids. It was the stress that
went along with it” (p. 20). Now if this distinction seems too stupid for
words, stick with me for a moment. Since
poverty is not the problem but, rather, the stress that goes along with it, and
since the stress is not the problem but, rather, the body’s reaction to it, it
stands to reform industry reason that changing the body’s reaction may now be
viewed as the way to short-circuit the effects of poverty. And, of course, to
change the body, we must change the mind, which is to say, we must change the
brain:
In the pursuit of “better
executive functioning,” Tough appears totally oblivious to the fact that KIPP
adds another form of stress atop the ones already at work on children living in
poverty. It seems, too, that Tough finds
nothing breath-taking about the neo-eugenic agenda of these new 21st
Century efficiency zealots, who prefer child brain tinkering to the more
expensive structural interventions and resource reallocations required for
addressing poverty. If KIPP and the KIPP
wannabe reform schools can “improve a child’s environment in the specific ways
that lead to better executive functioning [better memorization skills], then
who needs to be concerned, it would seem, that these new environments are
segregated, total compliance reform “families” run by young, white missionary
types who have been treated generously, themselves, to Seligman’s performance
character regimen during their six weeks of TFA teacher training. The quote
above certainly clears up what Tough meant back in the Introduction when he
waxed poetic about using “the tools of science to peel back the mysteries of
childhood” (p. xxiv).
In this entirely creepy form
of scientific reductionism practiced on children deemed defective by their
poverty, the sociological and psychological manifestations of poverty are
boiled down from actions to behaviors to neuronal reactions to molecular and
protein interactions. And these most
basic interactions are, then, alterable with the right “cognitive control
system” that can re-regulate anti-social urges by altering the brain chemistry
that controls them. If you think this
sounds too much like a variation on Clockwork
Orange, you would be right, and if you think that Paul Tough surely could
not be promoting such a system, I invite you to read the book.
In the end, Tough’s zeal for
pseudo-scientific psychological experimentation on children by
non-professionals is driven by his own misplaced anger on behalf of poor
children. As the angry do-gooder, then, he
represents the liberal element of the coalition in support of the new
eugenics. He is joined by the college
presidents such as liberal ethicist and president of UPenn, Amy Gutmann and
over twenty other Ivy League presidents who share Tough’s misdirected anger and
who have created a special category of affirmative action for former
KIPPsters. For conservatives who support
KIPP and the other learning chain gangs that emulate them, the goal of social
control is as transparent as was Paul Ryan’s calloused comments in Flint,
Michigan. For liberals like Tough,
however, who blame parents, teachers, and schools in these poorest
neighborhoods for failures that the inequitable system of schooling has
guaranteed and its high stakes testing perpetuates, the goal becomes masked by
a technicist indignation and a privileged arrogance that is used by liberals to
set themselves apart from the racist and classist system from which all of us
privileged folks benefit. And if Tough
could make the case that he, indeed, acknowledges his own role in the system that
we, the privileged, perpetuate, which would create the moral necessity to
change things for the better, he fails to keep in mind that children’s brains
are not things that are to be changed so that our economic privileges may
be kept inoculated from the required financial sacrifices that could help us
avoid, perhaps, the further rotting away of our moral fiber.
Teachers who have taught at
KIPP and who are sharing their stories know why children refer to KIPP as Kids
in Prison Program. They know about
enforced silence that keeps them from getting to know and connect with their
students. They know about their screaming
colleagues who rant and rave to maintain order, and they know about the
humiliation children are made to feel at even minor infractions of the rules. They know about barking orders to “Track Me,”
and they know about the constant
surveillance that leaves no peace for students or teachers. They know about the
special education children whose IEPs are ignored. They know, too about the management methods
that always made them feel as if the 70-90 hours they were giving each week was
not enough, and they know about the sense of personal weakness they felt on
those rare occasions when they had to use a sick day. They know all about the guilt they felt for
having a life outside KIPP. They know
about children who have been mistreated and abused, and they know about
administrators and teachers who lost jobs because their humanity got in the way
of rule enforcement to subdue children and to make them as hard as the psychological
catechism they are brainwashed to live by.
They know about the children whose emotional family histories kept them
from making it through the KIPP gauntlet, and they know those children blamed
themselves, rather than the draconian “no excuses” school model that provides
millionaire investors with tax breaks to fund the cheapest of all solutions to
the problems that arise from poverty.
I think that Paul Tough is
probably ignorant of all these things that teachers know, but I am not at all
sure he has any curiosity to find out what teachers, children, and parents
know. I do hope the next book that he is
paid to write about education reform schools goes beyond the search for
evidence to support some hare-brained theory meted out upon poor children in ways
that thoroughly displace the humane responsiveness and humility required to
help any children, poor or otherwise, to become whole adults, rather than
emotional eunuchs trained to perform, and not to think and feel.
Paul Tough, KIPP, and the Character Con: A Review of How Children Succeed: Grit, Curiosity, and the Hidden Power of Character
Jim Horn
Two days before the 2012 presidential election, Rachel Maddow ran a clip from an interview with uber-conservative, Paul Ryan, who not so long ago had plans, you might recall, to be Vice-President of the U. S. The interview was conducted by a local reporter in Flint, MI, one of the poorest and most violent urban areas of America. Ryan offered this suggestion for solving the crime and violence problems of Flint:When we look at these kids and their behavior, it can all seem so mysterious….But at some point, what you’re seeing is just a complex series of chemical reactions. It’s the folding of a protein or the activation of a neuron. And what’s exciting about that is that those things are treatable. When you get down to the molecules, you realize, that’s where the healing is. That’s where you’re discovering a solution” (p. 26). Paul Tough quoting Nadine Burke Harris
the best thing to help prevent violent
crime in the inner cities is to bring opportunity in the inner cities, is to
help people get out of poverty in the inner cities, is to help teach people
good discipline, good character. That is civil society.
Maddow and most other progressives were aghast when they
heard an Ayn Rand conservative like Paul Ryan say such a thing. Oddly, however, when a New York Times
writer/reporter draws the same conclusion, as Paul Tough does in his
bestseller, How Children Succeed: Grit,
Curiosity, and the Hidden Power of Character, the “progressive”
establishment is either silent as church mice or ready to throw contributions
(tax deductible) into the corporate offering plate to actively support school
programs like KIPP that are aimed to do just what Paul Ryan had the temerity to
admit, and for which we are rightfully indignant.
Tough’s book is out to promote an abusively-deployed variety of moral colonialism disguised as character building, even if he takes the most roundabout way to reach the conclusion from which he started, which may be stated thusly for those disinclined to read the book: KIPP and the education reform schools like it are doing the necessary work to save non-privileged, defective cultures from their own defective character, by making their children salable commodities in the future job market through a psychological regimen we would call brainwashing if it were used by a drug-crazed preacher in a faraway jungle like Guyana. Tough’s defense of the new eugenics is built upon a shaky collection of scientific tidbits and inappropriate analogies to other more humane interventions that Tough apparently believes are not unalike the pedagogical brutality and mind alteration practices that occur in the corporate reform schools such as KIPP. For instance, Tough analogizes from his experiences reporting on New York City’s chess champion school, IS 318, and through a number of examples of single-minded obsession paying off in the world of chess, suggests there is some lesson that may be transferred to understand KIPP’s 10,000 hours of total compliance test preparation and psychological throttling. As in other examples that Tough employs in his book, there are more differences between KIPP’s academic rigor mortis and becoming a chess champ than there are similarities.
The reason that researchers
who care about the gap between rich and poor are so excited about executive
functions is that these skills are not only highly predictive of success; they
are also quite malleable, much more so than other cognitive skills. The
prefrontal cortex is more responsive to intervention than other parts of the
brain, and it stays flexible well into adolescence and early adulthood. So if we can improve a child’s environment in
the specific ways that lead to better executive functioning, we can increase
his prospects for success in a particularly efficient way (p. 21).
Reference:
Tough,
P. (2012). How children
succeed: Grit, curiosity, and the hidden
power of character. New York:
Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.
Yes! Thank you for this. Tough's book rubbed me the wrong way too: http://atthechalkface.com/2012/09/30/paul-tough-is-way-off-base-and-stop-saying-grit/
ReplyDeleteWowser review, making critical points. The description is so accurate: "creepy form of scientific reductionism practiced on children deemed defective by their poverty." And the conclusion is one we can apply to most "reformers": Like Paul Tough, they are "probably ignorant of all these things that teachers know, but I am not at all sure he has any curiosity to find out what teachers, children, and parents know." They seem incapable of acknowledge that teachers, children, and parents know anything. And now is heartbreaking to see teachers fight against handing over what they know about how kids learn to the Common Core State (sic) Standards.
ReplyDeleteThank you, Jim.
ReplyDeleteThis review is important; it hits hard on the "creepy form of scientific reductionism practiced on children deemed defective by their poverty." Wonderfully accurate way to describe what's going on. Jim identifies the harm wrought by creepiness in detail, and he concludes something about Paul Tough that's probably true of many "reformers": "I think that Paul Tough is probably ignorant of all these things that teachers know, but I am not at all sure he has any curiosity to find out what teachers, children, and parents know." Very very few reformers care about what teachers, children, and parents know.
ReplyDeleteThis gets to the heart of it -- or rather the heartlessness of it. I suppose the Irish famine victims lacked "grit".
ReplyDelete