KIPP Model schools have long used Walter Mischel's research on delayed gratification among children to justify an indoctrination program aimed to manipulate economically-oppressed children to behave as corporate ed reformers would like: work hard, be nice, use self-control, and wait until your just rewards come to you, even if that happens to be NEVER. As I wrote in my 2016 book,
[t]he
philanthrocapitalists and their think tank scholars quote liberally from the
work of Walter Mischel (1989, 2014), whose experiments with delayed
gratification among preschoolers provide the dominant metaphor for another
generation of paternalist endeavors. In
Mischel’s experiments, children were offered a single marshmallow immediately
or two marshmallows later if they could delay their reward. The test, which came to be labeled “The
Marshmallow Test,” represents the potential to delay gratification in order to
gain a larger reward later on.
At many of the KIPP, Aspire,
Achievement First, and Yes Prep schools, children wear t-shirts emblazoned with
“Don’t Eat the Marshmallow.” Mischel’s (2014) latest work, The marshmallow test: Mastering self-control acknowledges KIPP’s
prominent role and places it within the context of recent research on improving
self-control. David Levin has made
Mischel’s book a central component in his Coursera massive open online course
(MOOC), Teaching character and creating
positive classrooms, which was first offered with co-instructor, Angela
Duckworth, in 2014.
The Atlantic reported last June on new research showing that Mischel's conclusions were flawed.
. . . .Ultimately, the new study finds limited support for the idea that being
able to delay gratification leads to better outcomes. Instead, it
suggests that the capacity to hold out for a second marshmallow is
shaped in large part by a child’s social and economic background—and, in
turn, that that background, not the ability to delay gratification, is
what’s behind kids’ long-term success. . . .
. . . .This new paper found that among kids whose
mothers had a college degree, those who waited for a second marshmallow
did no better in the long run—in terms of standardized test scores and
mothers’ reports of their children’s behavior—than those who dug right
in. Similarly, among kids whose mothers did not have college degrees,
those who waited did no better than those who gave in to temptation,
once other factors like household income and the child’s home
environment at age 3 (evaluated according to a standard research measure
that notes, for instance, the number of books that researchers observed
in the home and how responsive mothers were to their children in the
researchers’ presence) were taken into account. For those kids,
self-control alone couldn’t overcome economic and social disadvantages.
The failed replication of the marshmallow test
does more than just debunk the earlier notion; it suggests other
possible explanations for why poorer kids would be less motivated to
wait for that second marshmallow. For them, daily life holds fewer
guarantees: There might be food in the pantry today, but there might not
be tomorrow, so there is a risk that comes with waiting. And even if
their parents promise to buy more of a certain food, sometimes that
promise gets broken out of financial necessity.
Meanwhile, for kids who come from households
headed by parents who are better educated and earn more money, it’s
typically easier to delay gratification: Experience tends to tell them
that adults have the resources and financial stability to keep the
pantry well stocked. And even if these children don’t delay
gratification, they can trust that things will all work out in the
end—that even if they don’t get the second marshmallow, they can
probably count on their parents to take them out for ice cream instead.
There's another version run in Rochester in 2012 that shows similar conclusions
ReplyDeletehttp://curmudgucation.blogspot.com/2015/02/waiting-for-marshmallows.html